Publications
Refereed Scientific Papers in Journals
2024 | |||
---|---|---|---|
129. | H. Huygelier, N. Tuts, K. Michiels, E. Note, F. Schillebeeckx, J. Tournoy, V. vanden Abeele, R. van Ee & C.R. Gillebert (2024). The efficacy and feasibility of an immersive virtual reality game to train spatial attention orientation after stroke: a stage 2 report. J. of Neuropsychology, 00, 1-52. |
![]() |
|
2023 | |||
128. | A.P.M. Verhaegh, B.E. Groen, P.B.M. Aarts, R. van Ee, M.A.A.P. Willemsen, M.L.A. Jongsma & M.W.G. Nijhuis-van der Sanden (2023). Multisensory Stimulation and Priming (MuSSAP) in 4-10 Months Old Infants with a Unilateral Brain Lesion: A Pilot Randomised Clinical Trial. Occupational Therapy International, 8128407, 1-12. |
![]() |
|
2022 | |||
127. | S. Beumer, P. Boon, D.C.W. Klooster, R. van Ee, E. Carrette, M.M. Paulides & R.M.C. Mestrom (2022). Personalized tDCS for Focal Epilepsy — A Narrative Review: A Data-Driven Workflow Based on Imaging and EEG Data. Brain Sciences, 12, 610. |
![]() |
|
2021 | |||
126. | N. Kogo, F.B. Kern, T. Nowotny, R. van Ee, R. van Wezel & T. Aihara (2021). Dynamics of a Mutual Inhibition between Pyramidal Neurons Compared to Human Perceptual Competition. Journal of NeuroScience, 41, 1251–1264. |
![]() |
|
125. | H. Huygelier, R. van Ee, A. Lanssens, J. Wagemans, & C.R. Gillebert (2021). Audiovisual looming signals are not always prioritised: evidence from exogenous, endogenous and sustained attention. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 33, 282–303. |
![]() |
|
124. | V. vanden Abeele, B. Schraepen, H. Huygelier, C.R. Gillebert, K. Gerling & R. van Ee (2021). Immersive Virtual Reality for Older Adults: Empirically Grounded Design Guidelines. ACM Transactions on Accessible Computing, 14 (3,14), 1–30. |
![]() |
|
123. | H. Huygelier, E. Mattheus, V. vanden Abeele, R. van Ee, & C.R. Gillebert (2021). The Use of the Term Virtual Reality in Post-Stroke Rehabilitation: A Scoping Review and Commentary. Psychologica Belgica, 61, 145–162. |
![]() |
|
2020 | |||
122. | M.C. de Jong, M.J. Vansteensel, R. van Ee, F.S.S. Leijten, N.F. Ramsey, H.C. Dijkerman, S.O. Dumoulin & T. Knapen (2020). Intracranial recordings reveal unique shape and timing of responses in human visual cortex during illusory visual events. Current Biology,30, 3089–3100. |
![]() |
|
121. | J.H.D.M. Westerink, R.J.E. Rajae-Joordens, M. Ouwerkerk, M. van Dooren, S.M. Jelfs, A.J.M. Denissen, E. Penning de Vries & R. van Ee (2020). Deriving a cortisol-related stress indicator from wearable skin conductance measurements: Quantitative model & experimental validation. Front. Comput. Sci., 2:39, doi: 10.3389/fcomp.2020.00039. |
![]() |
|
120. | H. Huygelier, B. Schraepen, C. Lafosse, N. Vaes, F. Schillebeeckx, K. Michiels, E. Note, V. vanden Abeele, R. van Ee & C.R. Gillebert (2020). An immersive virtual reality game to train spatial attention orientation after stroke: a feasibility study. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, Sept 2020. |
![]() |
|
2019 | |||
119. | E. Arani, R. van Ee & R. van Wezel (2019). Age-dependency in binocular rivalry is reflected by exclusive percepts, not mixed percepts. Scientific Reports Nature, 9, 19271. |
![]() |
|
118. | H. Huygelier, B. Schraepen, R. van Ee, V. vanden Abeele & C.R. Gillebert (2019). Acceptance of immersive head-mounted virtual reality in older adults. Scientific Reports Nature, 9, 4519. |
![]() |
|
117. | M. Taranu, M.C. Wimmer, J. Ross, D. Farkas, R. van Ee, I. Winkler & S.L. Denham (2019). Children’s perception of visual and auditory ambiguity and its link to executive functions and creativity. Journal of experimental child psychology, 184, 123–138. |
![]() |
|
116. | S.K. Xu, Y. Chen, C.Y. Liu, G. Spekowius, R. van Ee, M. de Jong, M. Shen, Y. Li & J.G. Wang (2019). A randomized cross-over study on the blood pressure lowering effect of the combined passive head-up and -down movement with device-guided slow breathing. Blood Pressure, 28, 291–299. |
![]() |
|
115. | P. Moors, S. Gayet, N. Hedger, T. Stein, P. Sterzer, R. van Ee, J. Wagemans & G. Hesselmann (2019). Three criteria for evaluating high-Level processing in continuous flash suppression. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 267-269. |
![]() |
|
2018 | |||
114. | E. Arani, R. van Ee & R. van Wezel (2018). Changes in low-level neural properties underlie age-dependent visual decision making. Scientific Reports Nature, 8, 10789. | ![]() |
|
113. | S.L. Denham, D. Farkas, R. van Ee, M. Taranu, Z. Kocsis, M. Wimmer, D. Carmel & I. Winkler (2018). Similar but separate systems underlie perceptual bistability in vision and audition. Scientific Reports Nature, 8:7106. |
![]() |
112. | J.L. Souman, A.M. Tinga, S.F. te Pas, R. van Ee & B.N.S. Vlaskamp (2018). Acute alerting effects of light: A systematic literature review. Behavioural Brain Research, 337, 228-239. |
![]() |
2017 | |||
111. | P. Moors, G. Hesselmann, J. Wagemans & R. van Ee (2017). Continuous flash suppression: stimulus fractionation rather than integration. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 719-721. |
![]() |
|
110. | R. Dorman & R. van Ee (2017). 50 years of stereoblindness: Reconciliation of a continuum of disparity detectors with blindness for disparity in near or far depth. i-Perception, 1-13. |
![]() |
|
109. | T. de Graaf, R. van Ee, D. Croonenberg, P.C. Klink & A.T. Sack (2017). Visual suppression at the offset of binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 17(1):2, 1–18. |
![]() |
|
108. | F. Reinoso-Carvalho, Q. Wang, R. van Ee, D. Persoone & C. Spence (2017).“Smooth Operator”: Music modulates the perceived creaminess, sweetness, and bitterness of chocolate. Appetite, 108, 383-390. | ![]() |
|
107. | H. Huygelier, C.R. Gillebert, R. van Ee & V. vanden Abeele (2017). The design of a virtual reality game for stroke-induced attention deficits. In CHI PLAY '17 Annual Symposium on Computer-Human Interaction in Play (pp. 223-230). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: ACM, doi:10.1145/3130859.3131308. |
![]() |
|
106. | J. Janssen, O. Verschuren, W.J. Rengerd, J. Ermersa, M. Ketelaar & R. van Ee (2017). Gamification in physical therapy: more than using games. Pediatric Physical Therapy, 29, 95-99. | ![]() |
|
2016 | |||
105. | R. van Ee, S. Van de Cruys, L.J.M. Schlangen & B.N.S. Vlaskamp (2016). Circadian-Time Sickness: Time-of-day cue-conflicts directly affect health. Trends in Neurosciences, 39, 738-749. | ![]() |
|
Commentary article in Science News by L. Sanders with comments of Key Opinion Leaders. | ![]() |
||
104. | M. Vergeer, J. Wagemans & R. van Ee (2016). Training of binocular rivalry suppression suggests stimulus-specific plasticity in monocular and binocular visual areas. Scientific Reports Nature, 6:25753, doi: 10.1038/srep25753. | ![]() |
|
103. | M.C. de Jong, R.J.M. Hendriks, M.J. Vansteensel, M. Raemaekers, F.A.J. Verstraten, N.F. Ramsey, C.J. Erkelens, F.S.S. Leijten & R. van Ee (2016). Intracranial recordings of occipital cortex responses to illusory visual events. Journal of NeuroScience, 36(23), 6297-6311. | ![]() |
|
102. | F. Reinoso Carvalho, C. Y. Valasco, R. van Ee, Y. Leboeuf & C. Spence (2016). Music Influences Hedonic and Taste Ratings in Beer. Frontiers in Psychology, 7:636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00636 | ![]() |
|
101. | P. Moors, J. Wagemans, R. van Ee & L. de-Wit (2016). No evidence for surface organization in Kanizsa configurations during continuous flash suppression. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 78:902-914. | ![]() |
|
100. | M. Vergeer, P. Moors, J. Wagemans & R. van Ee (2016). Visible and invisible stimulus parts integrate into global object representations as revealed by combining monocular and binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 16(11):14, 1–10, doi:10.1167/16.11.14. | ![]() |
|
99. | F. Reinoso Carvalho, Q. Wang, B. de Causmaecker, K. Steenhaut, R. van Ee, & C. Spence (2016). Tune that Beer! Listening to the pitch of Beers. Bevarages, 2, 31, 1-11. | ![]() |
|
98. | K-E. Tsafou, J. LaCroix, R. van Ee, C.D.W. Vinkers & D.T.D. De Ridder (2016). The relation of trait and state mindfulness with satisfaction and physical activity: A cross-sectional study in 317 Dutch participants. Journal of Health Psychology, 1-12, DOI: 10.1177/1359105315624748. | ||
97. | F. Reinoso Carvalho, Q. Wang, R. van Ee & C. Spence (2016). The influence of soundscapes on the perception and evaluation of beers. Food Quality and Preference, 52, 32–41. | ![]() |
|
96. | A.M. Tinga, J.M.A. Visser-Meily, M.J. van der Smagt, S. van der Stigchel, R. van Ee, & T.C.W. Nijboer (2016). Multisensory stimulation to improve low- and higher-level sensory deficits after stroke: a systematic review. Neuropsychology Review, 26, 73–91, DOI 10.1007/s11065-015-9301-1. | ![]() |
2015 |
95. | P. Moors, T. Stein, J. Wagemans & R. van Ee (2015). Serial correlations in Continuous Flash Suppression. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 1-10, doi: 10.1093/nc/niv010. | ![]() |
|
94. | N. Kogo, L. Hermans, D Stuer, R. van Ee & Wagemans (2015). Temporal dynamics of bi-stable figure-ground perception: High-level feedback factors play a key role. Vision Research, 106, 7-19. | ![]() |
|
93. | F. Reinoso Carvalho, R. van Ee, M. Rychtarikova, A. Touhafi, K. Steenhaut, D. Persoone & C. Spence (2015). Using sound-taste correspondences to enhance the subjective value of tasting experiences. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:1309, 1-8. | ![]() |
|
92. | K-E. Tsafou, D.T.D. De Ridder, R. van Ee & J. LaCroix (2015). Mindfulness and satisfaction in physical activity: A cross-sectional study in the Dutch population. Journal of Health Psychology, 1-11, DOI: 10.1177/1359105314567207. | ||
91. | P. Moors, H. Huygelier, J. Wagemans, L. de-Wit & R. van Ee (2015). Suppressed visual looming stimuli are not integrated with auditory looming signals: Evidence from continuous flash suppression. i-Perception, 6(1), 48-62. | ![]() |
|
90. | F. Reinoso Carvalho, R. van Ee M. Rychtarikova, A. Touhafi, K. Steenhaut, D. Persoone, C. Spence, & M. Leman (2015). Does music influence the multisensory tasting experience? Journal of Sensory Studies, 30(5), 404-412. | ![]() |
|
89. | N. Kogo & R. van Ee (2015). Neural mechanisms of figure-ground organization: Border-ownership, competition and perceptual switching. In Oxford Handbook of Perceptual Organization (pp. 352-372). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | ||
2014 | |||
88. | M.C. de Jong, J.W. Brascamp, C. Kemner, R. van Ee & F.A.J. Verstraten (2014). Implicit perceptual memory modulates early visual processing of ambiguous images. Journal of NeuroScience, 34(30), 9970–9981. | ![]() |
|
2013 | |||
87. | T. Borra, H. Versnel, C. Kemner, A.J. van Opstal & R. van Ee (2013). An octave effect in auditory attention. PNAS, 110, 15225-15230. | ![]() |
|
86. | R. van Ee (2013). Cognition at Christmas: Preface to Special Issue. Learning & Perception, 5:suppl 2, 33-35. | ![]() |
|
85. | E.A. Miendlarzewska, G. van Elswijk, C.V. Cannistraci & R. van Ee (2013). Working memory load attenuates emotional enhancement in recognition memory. Frontiers in Psychology: Emotion Science, 4:112, 1-10. | ![]() |
|
84. | C. Klink, R.J.A. van Wezel & R. van Ee (2013). The future of binocular rivalry research: Reaching through a window on consciousness. In S.M. Miller (ed.), The Constitution of Visual Consciousness: Lessons from Binocular Rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research. John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 305-332. | ![]() |
|
2012 | |||
83. | M.C. de Jong, Z. Kourtzi & R. van Ee (2012). Perceptual experience modulates cortical circuits involved in visual awareness. European Journal of NeuroScience, 36, 3718–3731. | ![]() |
|
82. | C. Klink, R.J.A. van Wezel & R. van Ee (2012). United we sense, divided we fail: Context-driven perception of ambiguous visual stimuli. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 367 (SI 1591), 932-941. | ![]() |
|
81. | M.C. de Jong, T.H.J. Knapen & R. van Ee (2012). Opposite influence of perceptual memory on initial and prolonged perception of sensory ambiguity. PLoS ONE, 7(1):e30595. | ![]() |
|
2011 | |||
80. | T.H.J. Knapen, J. Pearson, J.W. Brascamp, R. van Ee & R. Blake (2011). Activity in frontal cortex during bistability reflects perceptual indecision. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(28):10293-10301. | ![]() |
|
79. | R. van Ee (2011). Percept-switch nucleation in binocular rivalry reveals local adaptation characteristics of early visual processing. Journal of Vision, 11(2):13, 1-12. http://journalofvision.org/11/2/13 |
![]() |
|
78. | T. de Graaf, M.C. de Jong, R. Goebel, R. van Ee & A.T. Sack (2011). On the Functional Relevance of Frontal Cortex for Passive and Voluntarily Controlled Bistable Vision. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10): 2322-2331. |
![]() |
|
2010 | |||
77. | R. van Ee, & C.J. Erkelens (2010). Stereo-vision: Head-centric coding of retina signals. Current Biology, 20, R567-568. | ![]() |
|
76. | J.W. Brascamp, R. Kanai, V. Walsh & R. van Ee (2010). Human Middle Temporal Cortex, perceptual bias, and perceptual memory for ambiguous Three-Dimensional motion. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 760-766. | ![]() |
|
75. | D.M. Alais, J.J.A. van Boxtel, A.L. Parker & R. van Ee (2010). Attending to auditory signals slows visual alternations in binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 50, 929-935. | ![]() |
|
74. | J.J. Koenderink, L. Albertazzi, A.J. van Doorn, R. van Ee, et al. (2010). Does monocular space contain planes? Acta Psychologica, 134, May, 40-47. | ![]() |
|
73. | D.A. Wismeijer, C.J. Erkelens, R. van Ee & M. Wexler (2010). Depth cue combination in spontaneous eye movements. Journal of Vision, 10(6):25, 1-15. | ![]() |
|
72. | F.L. Kooi, D. Dekker, R. van Ee & A. Brouwer (2010). Real 3D increases perceived depth over anaglyphs but does not cancel stereo-anomaly. Displays, 31, 132-138. | ![]() |
|
2009 | |||
71. | R. van Ee, J.J.A. van Boxtel, A.L. Parker & D.M. Alais (2009). Multisensory congruency as a mechanism for attentional control over perceptual selection. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 11641-11649. | ![]() | |
See icon to the far most right for Supplementary material. | ![]() |
||
70. | R. van Ee (2009). Stochastic variations in sensory awareness are driven by noisy neuronal adaptation: Evidence from serial correlations in perceptual bistability. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 26, 2612-2622. | ![]() |
|
69. | G.J. Brouwer, F. Tong, P. Hagoort & R. van Ee (2009). Perceptual incongruence influences bistability and cortical activation. PLoS ONE, 4(3): e5056. | ![]() |
|
68. | M. Raemaekers, M.E. van der Schaaf, R. van Ee & R.J.A. van Wezel (2009). Widespread fMRI activity differences between perceptual states in rivalry are correlated with differences in observer biases. Brain Research, 1252, 161-171. | ![]() |
|
2008 | |||
67. | C. Klink, R. van Ee & R.J.A. van Wezel (2008). General Validity of Levelt's Propositions Reveals Common Computational Mechanisms for Visual Rivalry. PLoS ONE, 3(10), e3473. | ![]() |
|
66. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, T. Knapen, C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2008). Removal of monocular interactions equates rivalry behavior for monocular, binocular, and stimulus rivalries. Journal of Vision, 8(15):13, 1–17. | ![]() |
|
65. | A.P. Kamphuisen, M, Bauer & R. van Ee (2008). No evidence for widespread synchronized networks in binocular rivalry: MEG frequency tagging entrains primarily early visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 8(5):4, 1-8. http://journalofvision.org/8/5/4 | ![]() |
|
64. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, D.M. Alais, C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2008). The role of temporally coarse form processing during binocular rivalry. PLoS ONE, 3(1): e1429. | ![]() |
|
63. | C. Klink, R. van Ee, M.M. Nijs, G.J. Brouwer, A.J. Noest & R.J.A. van Wezel (2008). Early interactions between neuronal adaptation and voluntary control determine perceptual choices in bistable vision. Journal of Vision, 8(5):16, 1-18. http://journalofvision.org/8/5/16 | ![]() |
|
62. | J.W. Brascamp, T.H.J. Knapen, R. Kanai, A.J. Noest, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2008). Multi-timescale perceptual history resolves visual ambiguity. PLoS ONE, 3(1): e1497. | ![]() |
|
61. | D.A. Wismeijer, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2008). Depth cues, rather than perceived depth, govern vergence. Experimental Brain Research, 184, 61-70. | ![]() |
|
60. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, D.M. Alais & R. van Ee (2008). Retinotopic and non-retinotopic stimulus encoding in binocular rivalry and the involvement of feedback. Journal of Vision, 8(5):17, 1-10. http://journalofvision.org/8/5/17 | ![]() |
|
2007 | |||
59. | G.J. Brouwer & R. van Ee (2007). Visual cortex allows prediction of perceptual states during ambiguous structure-from-motion. Journal of NeuroScience, 27, 1015-1023. | ![]() |
|
58. | T.H.J. Knapen, R. van Ee & R. Blake (2007). Stimulus motion propels traveling waves in binocular rivalry. PLoS ONE, 2(8): e739. | ![]() |
|
57. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2007). Dichoptic masking and binocular rivalry share common perceptual dynamics. Journal of Vision, Journal of Vision, 7(14):3, 1-11. | ![]() |
|
56. | T.H.J. Knapen, R. Kanai, J.W. Brascamp, J.J.A. van Boxtel & R. van Ee (2007). Distance in feature space determines exclusivity in visual rivalry. Vision Research, 47, 3269-3275. | ![]() |
|
55. | J.W. Brascamp, T.H.J. Knapen, R. Kanai, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2007). Flash suppression and flash facilitation in binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 7(12):12,1-12. | ![]() |
|
54. | R. Kanai, T.H.J. Knapen, R. van Ee & F.A.J. Verstraten (2007). Disruption of implicit perceptual memory by intervening neutral stimuli. Vision Research, 47, 2675-2683. | ![]() |
|
53. | A.J. Noest, R. van Ee, M.M. Nijs & R.J.A. van Wezel (2007). Percept-choice sequences driven by interrupted ambiguous stimuli: A low-level neural model. Journal of Vision, 7(8):10, 1-14. | ![]() |
|
52. | A.P. Kamphuisen, R.J.A. van Wezel & R. van Ee (2007). Inter-ocular transfer of stimulus cueing in dominance selection at the onset of binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 47, 1142-1144. | ![]() |
|
51. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2007). Monocular symmetry in binocular vision. Journal of Vision, 7(4):5,1-9. | ![]() |
|
50. | T.H.J. Knapen, C. Paffen, R. Kanai & R. van Ee (2007). Stimulus flicker alters interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 47, 1-7. | ![]() |
|
2006 | |||
49. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2006). A single system explains human speed perception. Journal of Cognitive NeuroScience, 18, 1808-1819. | ![]() |
|
48. | J.W. Brascamp, R. van Ee, A.J. Noest, R.H.A.H. Jacobs & A.V. van den Berg (2006). The time course of binocular rivalry reveals a fundamental role of noise. Journal of Vision, 6, 1244-1256. | ![]() |
|
47. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2006). Retinal image shifts, but not eye movements per se, cause alternations in awareness during binocular rivalry. Journal of vision, 6, 1172-1179. | ![]() |
|
46. | A. Noest, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2006). Direct extraction of curvature-based metric shape from stereo. Biological Cybernetics, 95, 455-486. | ![]() |
|
45. | R. van Ee, A.J. Noest, J.W. Brascamp & A.V. van den Berg (2006). Attentional control over either of the two competing percepts of ambiguous stimuli revealed by a two-parameter analysis: Means don't make the difference. Vision Research, 46, 3129-3141. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.017. | ![]() |
|
44. | G.J. Brouwer & R. van Ee (2006). Endogenous influences on perceptual bistability depend on exogenous stimulus characteristics. Vision Research, 46, 3393-3402. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.016. | ![]() |
|
43. | T.H.J. Knapen & R. van Ee (2006). Slant perception, and its voluntary control, do not govern the slant aftereffect: multiple slant signals adapt independently. Vision Research, 46, 3381-3392. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.027. | ![]() |
|
42. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2006). The role of saccades in exerting voluntary control in perceptual and binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 46, 787-799. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.10.011. | ![]() |
|
2005 | |||
41. | G.J. Brouwer, R. van Ee & J. Schwarzbach (2005). Activation in visual cortex correlates with the awareness of stereoscopic depth. Journal of NeuroScience, 25, 10403-10413. | ![]() |
|
40. | H.E.M. den Ouden, R. van Ee & E. de Haan (2005). Colour helps to solve the binocular matching problem. Journal of Physiology, 567.2, 665-671. | ![]() |
|
39. | R. van Ee, G. Krumina, S.P. Pont & S. van der Ven (2005). Voluntarily controlled bi-stable slant perception of real and photographed surfaces. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B, 272, 141-148. | ![]() |
|
38. | J.W. Brascamp, R. van Ee, W.R. Pestman & A.V. van den Berg (2005). Distributions of alternation rates in various forms of bistable perception. Journal of Vision, 5, 287-298. | ![]() |
|
37. | R. van Ee (2005). Dynamics of perceptual bi-stability for stereoscopic slant rivalry and a comparison with grating, house-face, and Necker cube rivalry. Vision Research, 45, 29-40. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.039. | ![]() |
|
36. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2005). The role of (micro)saccades and blinks in perceptual bi-stability from slant rivalry. Vision Research, 45, 2417-2435. | ![]() |
|
35. | R. van Ee, L.C.J. van Dam & G.J. Brouwer (2005). Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability. Vision Research, 45, 41-55. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.030. | ![]() |
|
34. | A.V. van den Berg, R. van Ee & A.J. Noest (2005). Mixed visual reference frames: perceiving nonretino-centric visual quantities in a retino-centric frame. Proc. SPIE, Human Vision and Electronic Imaging (Eds: B.E. Rogowitz, T.N. Pappas, S.J. Daly), 5666, 449-461. | ![]() |
|
2004 | |||
33. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2004). Stereoscopic matching and the aperture problem. Perception, 33, 769-787. | ![]() |
|
2003 | |||
32. | R. van Ee (2003). Correlation between stereoanomaly and perceived depth when disparity and motion interact in binocular matching. Perception, 32, 67-84. | ![]() |
|
31. | K. Hol, A. Koene & R. van Ee (2003). Attention-biased multi-stable surface perception in three-dimensional structure-from-motion. Journal of Vision, 3, 486-498. | ![]() |
|
30. | R. van Ee, W.J. Adams & P. Mamassian (2003). Bayesian modeling of cue interaction: bi-stability in stereoscopic slant perception. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 20, 1398-1406. | ![]() |
|
29. | M.H. Both, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2003). Perceived slant from Werner's illusion affects binocular eye movements. Journal of Vision, 3, 685-697. | ![]() |
|
28. | R. van Ee & L.C.J. van Dam (2003). The influence of cyclovergence on unconstrained stereoscopic matching. Vision Research, 43, 307-319. | ![]() |
|
Assistent Professor Physics Utrecht (2000-2002) | |||
2002 | |||
27. | R. van Ee, L.C.J. van Dam & C.J. Erkelens (2002). Bi-stability in perceived slant when binocular disparity and monocular perspective specify different slants. Journal of Vision, 2, 597-607. | ![]() |
|
26. | E.M. Berends, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2002). Vertical disparity can alter perceived direction. Perception, 31, 1323-1333. | ![]() |
|
25. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2002). Multi-coloured stereograms unveil two binocular colour mechanisms in human vision. Vision Research, 42, 1103-1112. | ![]() |
|
24. | R. van Ee & W. Richards (2002). A planar and a volumetric test for stereoanomaly. Perception, 31, 51-64. | ![]() |
|
23. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2002). The role of the cyclopean eye in vision: sometimes inappropriate, always irrelevant. Vision Research, 42, 1157-1163. | ![]() |
|
Postdoctoral period MIT (1999-2000) | |||
2001 | |||
22. | R. van Ee & B.L. Anderson (2001). Motion direction, speed, and orientation in binocular matching. Nature, 410, 690-694. | ![]() |
|
21. | W.J. Adams, M.S. Banks & R. van Ee (2001). Adaptation to three-dimensional distortions in human vision. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 1063-1064. | ![]() |
|
20. | R. van Ee (2001). Perceptual learning without feedback and the stability of stereoscopic slant estimation. Perception, 30, 95-114. | ![]() |
|
19. | R. van Ee, B.L. Anderson & H. Farid (2001). Occlusion junctions do not improve stereoacuity. Spatial Vision, 15, (# 1, Nov 2001), 45-59. | ![]() |
|
Postdoctoral period UC Berkeley (1996-1999) | |||
2000 | |||
18. | R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2000). Is there an interaction between perceived direction and perceived aspect ratio in stereoscopic vision? Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 910-926. | ![]() |
|
17. | R. van Ee & C.M. Schor (2000). Unconstrained stereoscopic matching of lines. Vision Research, 40, 151-162. | ![]() |
|
1999 | |||
16. | R. van Ee, M.S. Banks & B.T. Backus (1999). An analysis of stereoscopic slant contrast. Perception, 28, 1121-1145. | ![]() |
|
15. | R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1999). The influence of large scanning eye movements on stereoscopic slant estimation of large surfaces. Vision Research, 39, 467-479. | ![]() |
|
14. | B.T. Backus, M.S. Banks, R. van Ee & J.A. Crowell (1999). Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic slant perception. Vision Research, 39, 1143-1170. | ![]() |
|
13. | R. van Ee, M.S. Banks & B.T. Backus (1999). Perceived visual direction near an occluder. Vision Research, 39, 4085-4097. | ![]() |
|
1998 | |||
12. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (1998). A computational model of depth perception based on headcentric disparity. Vision Research, 38, 2999-3018. | ||
11. | R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1998). Temporal aspects of stereoscopic slant estimation: An evaluation and extension of Howard and Kaneko's theory. Vision Research, 38, 3871-3882. | ![]() |
|
1997 | |||
10. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (1997). Capture of the visual direction of monocular objects by adjacent binocular objects. Vision Research, 37, 1735-1745. | ||
9. | M.S. Banks, R. van Ee & B.T. Backus (1997). The computation of binocular visual direction: A re-examination of Mansfield and Legge (1997). Vision Research, 37, 1605-1610. | ||
8. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (1997). Capture of visual direction: An unexpected phenomenon in binocular vision. Vision Research, 37, 1193-1196. | ||
Graduate Student Physics | |||
1996 | |||
7. | R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1996). Stability of binocular depth perception with moving head and eyes. Vision Research, 36, 3827-3842. | ![]() |
|
6. | R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1996). Anisotropy in Werner's binocular depth-contrast effect. Vision Research, 36, 2253-2262. | ![]() |
|
5. | R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1996). Temporal aspects of binocular slant perception. Vision Research, 36, 43-51. | ![]() |
|
4. | C.J. Erkelens, A.J.M. Muijs & R. van Ee (1996). Binocular alignment in different depth planes. Vision Research, 36, 2141-2147. | ||
1995 | |||
3. | R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1995). Binocular perception of slant about oblique axes relative to a visual frame of reference. Perception, 24, 299-314. | ![]() |
|
2. | PhD Thesis (August 1991 - November 1994; Utrecht University,
Dept Physics & Astronomy) R. van Ee (1995). Stability of binocular depth perception. ISBN 90-393-0846-2. |
||
1992 | |||
1. | R. Bhagwandien, R. van Ee, R. Beersma, C.J.G. Bakker, M.A. Moerland & J.J.W. Lagendijk (1992). Numerical analysis of the magnetic field for arbitrary magnetic susceptibility distributions in 2D. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 10, 299-313. |
Non-Vision Publications being Undergraduate Student work in Physics: |
|
1. | R. Bhagwandien, R. van Ee, R. Beersma, C.J.G. Bakker, M.A. Moerland & J.J.W. Lagendijk (1992). Numerical analysis of the magnetic field for arbitrary magnetic susceptibility distributions in 2D. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 10, 299-313. | ||
2. | R. van Ee, R. Bhagwandien, C. J. G. Bakker (1991). Improved analysis of susceptibility artifacts in NMR SE maging of simple objects. Proceedings of the 8th annual meeting of the European Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine and Biology (ESMRMB), page 248. | ||
3. | R. Bhagwandien, C. J. G. Bakker, R. van Ee, M. A. Moerland and J.J.W. Lagendijk (1991). Numerical analysis of the magnetic field for arbitrary magnetic susceptibility distributions. Proceedings of the 8th annual meeting of the European Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine and Biology (ESMRMB), page 250. | ||
Instruction readers | |||
4. | R. van Ee (1992). Analysis of susceptibility artefacts in magnetic resonance spin-echo imaging of simple objects. 3DCV 92-02. | ||
5. | R. van Ee (1992). Introduction to the fundamentals of magnetic resonance imaging. UBI-M.92.CV-030. | ||
6. | R. van Ee (1992). Analysis of susceptibility artefacts in magnetic resonance spin-echo imaging of simple objects. UBI-M.92.CV-035. |
Publications in Dutch Journals and Popular Scientific Magazins
- Out-of-sync body clock causes more woes than sleepiness by L. Sanders, Science News; October 2016.
- Cyclops Mirror, Science; Vol 326, p777, November 2009.
Dutch Journals:
- R. van Ee (1999). Gezichtsbedrog onthult hersenwerking
in driedimensionale waarneming. Nederlands
Tijdschrift voor Natuurkunde, 65/3, 65-69.
- R. van Ee & Verstraten (1999). Ons brein en
het waarnemen van diepte: nieuwe inzichten door een oude illusie. Nederlands
Tijdschrift voor Psychologie, 54, 159-166.
- R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (1994). Het waarnemen van diepte met twee bewegende ogen. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Natuurkunde, 60/20, 311-315.
Dutch and foreign newspapers: A number of reports about the research have been published.
- Interview for ‘EenVandaag’, national radio (NPO1), 25 Oct 2024.
- News papers: VolksKrant 6 Juli, 2024, 3 august 2024 heart-brain connection.
- News papers: Trouw 6 Apr, 2024, Lente in je Bol.
- Interview 'EenVandaag’, national radio (NPO1), 12 Feb 2020.
- Interview ‘Dit is de dag’, national radio (NPO1), 21 Oct 2019.
- Invited TV interview for TopDoks, national television (NPO3), 7 January 2018; 28 April, 2019.
- Invited TV commentary in news flash for RTL4, national television, Editie NL, 11 April 2017.
- Invited TV interview for VPRO, national television (NPO1), 11 May 2017.
- News papers: Volskrant 22 feb, 2017, music and chocolat.
- Radio broadcast: 24-25 feb 2017, VPRO "nooit meer slapen", music and chocolat.
- Radio broadcast: 22 feb2017, 3FM, music and chocolat.
- Radio broadcast: Feb 2017, Radio Noord Holland musick and chocolat.
- Television: 24 maart, 2017 TV Overijsel, bier en muziek
- News papers: Volskrant fall, 2016, circadian rhythm.
- Waarom voel je je onderbuik bij turbulentie? NRC next, 19 May 2011.
- Waarom val je in de trein in slaap? NRC next, 26 April 2011.
- Je eigen onafwendbare oog, Akademie nieuws, July 2008; December 2009.
- Hoe platte beelden 3-d worden. Utrechts nieuwsblad, 5 February 2005.
- Twijfelende hersenen brengen bewustzijn in beeld. U-blad, 25 April 2002.
- Drie-dimensionale waarneming, signalen en mechanismen: een hersenmodel. Vakidioot 1-4, May 2001.
- Bramen plukken stelt eisen aan de hersenen. U-blad, May 2001.
- Snelheid nodig voor 3D. Intermediair, May 2001.
- Hersenen gebruiken snelheid voor driedimensionale waarneming. UU, Persbericht, April 2001.
- Optische illusies onthullen principes van driedimensionaal zien. NWO, Nieuwsbrief media no. 8, Oct 1998.
- Stability of binocular depth perception. Vereniging voor Biofysica, no. 95, June 1995.
- Stability of binocular depth perception. Persberichten Universiteit Utrecht, no. 11, March 1995.
- Eye-opener in driedimensionaal zien. Bionieuws no. 14, September 1994.
- Hoe leer je een robot kijken? KIJK no. 4, April 1994.
- Une découverte réalisée dans la vision tridimensionelle rend possible la construction de robots de navigation. Bulletin de Recherche no. 3.3, June 1994.
- Better understanding of 3D vision allows robots to navigate. Research Reports from the Netherlands no. 4.3, June 1994.
- Neue Erkenntnisse über räumliches Sehen machen navigierende Roboter möglich. Forschungsnachrichten aus den Niederlanden no. 3.3, June 1994.
- 3D vision discovery will lead to better robots. Sensor Technology, no. 9, September 1994.
- Vondst in drie-dimensionaal zien maakt navigerende robots mogelijk. NWO, Nieuwsbrief voor de media no. 4, April 1994.
- Vondst 3D-zien bij mensen komt robotica te hulp. Technisch Weekblad no. 17, April 1994.
- Navigierende Roboter lernen vom Menschen. Wissenschaft, Politik, Wirtschaft no. 24, June 1994.
Refereed Abstracts
2010 | |
---|---|
107. | M.C. de Jong, et al, R. van Ee (2010). Occipital reflections of perceptual decisions revealed by subdural electrode recordings in human. Society for NeuroScience, Submitted. |
106. | T. Knapen, J.W. Brascamp, J. Pearson, R. van Ee, R. Blake (2010). Perceptual indecision during perceptual bistability: the role of right frontal cortex. Society for NeuroScience, Submitted. |
105. | T. de Graaf, M.C. de Jong, R. Goebel, R. van Ee, A.T. Sack (2010). The Brain changing its Mind: bistable perception and voluntary control investigated with frontoparietal TMS. Human Brain Mapping, Submitted. |
104. | R. van Ee (2010). Zero correlation is not a hallmark of perceptual bistability: Variation in percept duration is driven by noisy neuronal adaptation. Journal of Vision, 10(7):340. |
103. | D.A. Wismeijer, C.J. Erkelens, R. van Ee & M. Wexler (2010). Depth cue combination in spontaneous eye movements. Journal of Vision, 10(7):76. |
102. | M.C. de Jong, Z. Kourtzi, R. van Ee (2010). Perceptual memory increases amplitude of neural response in sensory brain regions. Journal of Vision, 10(7):343. |
101. | T. de Graaf, M.C. de Jong, R. van Ee, A.T. Sack (2010). The Brain changing its Mind: bistable perception and voluntary control investigated with frontoparietal TMS. Journal of Vision, 10(7):349. |
2009 | |
100. | H. Kroger & R. van Ee (2009). Percept switches in binocular rivalry: Dynamics of nucleation and domain walls. Frontiers of Fundamental Physics 10 (FFP10, Perth, Australia), 44. |
99. | L.I. Leiros, R. van Ee, F. Valle-Inclan & M.J. Blanco (2009). Individual differences in fixation eye movements: it is true that microsaccades are strictly binocular? ECEM, PB13:145. |
98. | D.A. Wismeijer, R. van Ee, C.J. Erkelens & M. Wexler (2009). 3D cue combination in spontaneous eye movements. Journal of Vision, 9(8):43. |
97. | M.C. de Jong & R. van Ee (2009). Perceptual Memory influences both continuous and intermittent Ambiguous Perception, but in opposite ways. Journal of Vision, 9(8):273. |
96. | J.W. Brascamp, R. Kanai, V. Walsh & R. van Ee (2009). The influence of TMS over MT on perceptual memory in structure-from-motion rivalry. Journal of Vision, 9(8):263. | 2008 |
95. | R. van Ee (2008). Reported evidence for widespread synchronized networks in visual rivalry is unconvincing. Perception, 37, S128. |
94. | M.C. de Jong, J.W. Brascamp, C. Kemner & R. van Ee (2008). Neural Correlates of Memory in Ambiguous Perception measured with EEG. Perception, 37, S155. |
93. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2008). Binocular rivalry from asynchronously flickering images: the temporal limit depends on trial duration and attention. Perception, 37, S100. |
92. | P.C. Klink, R. van Ee & R.J.A. van Wezel (2008). Levelt’s Propositions Generalized for Bistable Structure-from-Motion: Common Computational Mechanisms of Visual Rivalry. Perception, 37, S28. |
91. | A.J. Noest, R. van Ee & R. van Wezel (2008). Timescale-invariant neural response properties yield ‘stack’-memory behaviour. Perception, I37, S50. |
90. | T. Knapen, J. Pearson, J.W. Brascamp, R. van Ee & R. Blake (2008). The role of frontal areas in alternations during perceptual bistability. Journal of Vision, 8(6):254. |
89. | D.A. Wismeijer, T. Knapen, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2008). Influence of perspective and disparity on vergence smooth pursuit. Journal of Vision, 8(6):671. |
88. | R. van Ee (2008). Early neural interactions can explain perceptual bi-stability modifications of stimulus timing, perceptual history, cross-modal influence and attentional control. Journal of Vision, 8(6):794. | 2007 |
87. | R. van Ee, A.L. Parker, J.J.A. van Boxtel & D.M. Alais (2007). Voluntary control over visual rivalry is enhanced by sound that is consistent with scene interpretation. Society for NeuroScience, 126.10. |
86. | T. Knapen, J.J.A. van Boxtel & R. van Ee (2007). Neural circuitry of segregation and binding in perceptual rivalry. Society for NeuroScience, 201.10. |
85. | J.W. Brascamp, T. Knapen, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2007). History dependence on multiple timescales in ambiguous figure perception. Society for NeuroScience, 716.16. |
84. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2007). Binocular rivalry depends on temporally coarse form processing. Society for NeuroScience, 716.15. |
83. | P.C. Klink, R. van Ee, M.M. Nijs, A.J. Noest & R.J.A. van Wezel (2007). Early interactions between stimulus timing, perceptual history and voluntary control determine perceptual decisions in bi-stable vision. Society for NeuroScience, 507.14. |
82. | R. van Ee, A.L. Parker, J.J.A. van Boxtel & D.M. Alais (2007). Attending to auditory signals slows binocular rivalry. Perception, 36, S56. |
81. | D.M. Alais, A.L. Parker, J.J.A. van Boxtel, C. Paffen & R. van Ee (2007). Attending to auditory signals slows binocular rivalry. Perception, 36, S56. |
80. | T. Knapen, R. van Ee & R. Blake (2007). Stimulus motion propels traveling waves in binocular rivalry. Perception, 36, S56. |
79. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2007). Binocular rivalry for temporally modulated stimuli depends on a form-based parsing mechanism. Perception, 36, S56. |
78. | J.W. Brascamp, T. Knapen, N.V. Stolk, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2007). Temporal characteristics of history-dependence in ambiguous figure perception. Perception, 36, S45. |
77. | A.J. Noest, R. van Ee, R.J.A. van Wezel, P.C. Klink & M.M. Nijs, (2007). Bistable percept-choice dynamics explained by early interactions between stimulus timing, voluntary bias and perceptual history. Perception, 36, S143. |
76. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2007). The role of saccadic eye movements in perceptual bistability. German Gesellschaft für Kognitionswissenschaften, In Press. |
75. | G.J. Brouwer & R. van Ee (2007). Decoding visual awareness and its voluntary control during ambiguous structure-from-motion. Journal of Vision, 7 (9), 747. |
74. | T. Knapen, J. Pearson, R. Blake, & R. van Ee (2007). Increase of perceived speed accompanying onset of interocular suppression. Journal of Vision, 7 (9), 52. |
73. | J.W. Brascamp, T. Knapen, R. Kanai, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2007). Perceptual memory of ambiguous figures survives spontaneous perceptual alternations. Journal of Vision, 7 (9), 802. |
72. | D.A. Wismeijer, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2007). Influence of perceived depth in a reverse perspective stimulus on vergence. Journal of Vision, 7 (9), 28. |
71. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, T. Knapen, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2007). Identical rivalry dynamics for monocular, stimulus and binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 7 (9), 54. |
70. | L.C.J. van Dam, R. Mulder, A.J. Noest, J.W. Brascamp, A.V. van den Berg, & R. van Ee (2007). Sequential dependency in percept durations for binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 7 (9), 53. |
2006 | |
69. | G.J. Brouwer, R. van Ee & F. Tong (2006). Neural mechanisms underlying ambiguous structure-from-motion and voluntary control revealed by functional imaging. Association for the scientific study of consciousness, 10. |
68. | R.J.A van Wezel, A.J. Noest, M.M. Nijs & R. van Ee (2006). Percept choices at the onset of ambiguous or binocular rivalry stimuli. Journal of the Vision Society of Japan, 18, S156. |
67. | J.W. Brascamp, A.J. Noest, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2006). Transition phases show the importance of noise in binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 6(6), S845. |
66. | R. van Ee, J.W. Brascamp, G.J. Brouwer & L.C.J. van Dam (2006). Endogenous control over either of the two percepts that compete for visual awareness in perceptual rivalry. Association for the scientific study of consciousness, 10. |
65. | R. van Ee (2006). Attentional control over either of the two competing percepts for the Necker Cube. Journal of the Vision Society of Japan, 18, S50. |
64. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, A.P. Kamphuisen, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2006). The occurrence of binocular rivalry and dichoptic masking depends on temporal aspects of stimulator. Journal of Vision, 6(6), S850. |
63. | A.J. Noest, R. van Ee & R.J.A. van Wezel (2006). Visual choice dynamics: Explaining repetition and predicting alternation of bistable percepts driven by stimulus On/Off timing. Journal of Vision, 6(6), S51. |
62. | R. van Ee & T. Knapen (2006). Stereoscopic surface slant adaptation occurs before slant awareness: multiple slant signals adapt independently. Journal of Vision, 6(6), S647. |
61. | T. Knapen, C. Paffen, R. Kanai & R. van Ee (2006). Stimulus flicker alters interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 6(6), S49. |
60. | D.A. Wismeijer, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2006). The influence of perceived surface slant of an ambiguous stimulus on vergence. Perception, 35, S26. |
59. | T. Knapen, M. Struiksma & R. van Ee (2006). Multiple slant rivalry stimuli alternate independently. Perception, 35, S30. |
58. | J.J.A. van Boxtel, T. Knapen, R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (2006). Exploring the parameter space of stimulus rivalry. Perception, 35, S32. |
57. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2006). Saccadic retinal shifts correlate with perceptual alternations in binocular rivalry. Perception, 35, S25. |
56. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2006). Bilateral symmetry in binocular visioen. Perception, 35, S10. |
55. | R.J.A van Wezel, A.J. Noest, M.M. Nijs & R. van Ee (2006). Percept choices at the onset of ambiguous or binocular rivalry stimuli. Perception, 35, S29. |
54. | A.J. Noest & R. van Ee (2006). Statistical mechanics modelling of rivalrous dominance- and transition-durations. Perception, 35, S53. |
53. | R. van Ee, J.W. Brascamp, A.J. Noest & A.V. van den Berg (2006). Attentional control over either of the two competing percepts for the Necker Cube. Perception. 35, S162. |
52. | C.J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2006). Metric of binocular visual direction in stereopsis. Journal of Vision, 6(6), S424. |
51. | J.W. Brascamp, R. van Ee, A.J. Noest & A.V. van den Berg (2006). Temporal dynamics of binocular rivalry imply an important role for noise. Perception, 35, S9. |
2005 | |
50. | G.J. Brouwer, F. Tong, J. Schwarzbach & R. van Ee (2005). Voluntary control over perceptual changes during bistable Structure-from-Motion, revealed by fMRI. Society for NeuroScience, 664.13. |
49. | T. Knapen & R. van Ee (2005). Directed perceptual awareness does not influence slant adaptation under conditions of slant rivalry. Society for NeuroScience, 664.13. |
48. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2005). The role of version and vergence in visual bistability. Perception, 34, S145. |
47. | R. van Ee (2005). Visual awareness and voluntary control. Journal of Vision, 5(8), S707. |
46. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2005). The role of eye movements in bistability from perceptual and binocular rivalry and the role of voluntary control. Journal of Vision, 5(8), S704. |
2004 | |
45. | R. van Ee (2004). Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability. Australian Journal of Psychology, 56, S139. |
44. | J. Brascamp, A.V. van den Berg & R. van Ee (2004). Shared neural circuitry for switching between perceptual states and ocular motor states? Journal of Vision, in press, VSS abstract A29. |
43. | T. Knapen & R. van Ee (2004). Effects of attention on slant aftereffects in bi-stable stereoscopic depth perception. Perception, 33, S97. |
42. | A. Koene & R. van Ee (2004). Modelling bi-stability in perceived slant when binocular disparity and monocular perspective specify different slants. Perception, 33, S86. |
41. | G.J. Brouwer, F. Tong, J. Schwarzbach & R. van Ee (2004). Neural correlates of stereoscopic depth perception in visual cortex. Society for NeuroScience, 664.13. |
40. | R. van Ee (2004). Voluntary control and awareness of stereopsis. Progress in Biochemistry and Biophysics, 31, S47. |
2003 | |
39. | A. Koene & R. van Ee (2003). Modeling bi-stability in perceived slant. Journal of Physiology - Paris, in press. |
38. | R. van Ee, L.C.J. van Dam & G.J. Brouwer (2003). Attention-driven conscious bi-stable stereoscopic depth perception. Society for NeuroScience, 339.1. |
37. | G.J. Brouwer & R. van Ee (2003). The neural correlates of bistable stereoscopic depth perception: an fMRI study. Society for NeuroScience, 179.13. |
36. | A.J. Noest, R. van Ee & A.V. van den Berg (2003). Disentangling retinal and head-centric disparity-coding involved in perception of metric depth from stereo. Perception, 32, S13. |
35. | R. van Ee (2003). Attention-driven conscious bi-stable stereoscopic depth perception. Perception,32, S45. |
34. | L.C.J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2003). Bistability in stereoscopically perceived slant about a horizontal axis. Journal of Vision, 3(9), 467. |
33. | R. van Ee, L.C.J. van Dam, G.J. Brouwer & N.J.H. Korsten (2003). Bistable stereoscopic 3D percepts: Will-power, flip frequency, eye movements and blinks. Journal of Vision, 3(9), 160. |
2002 | |
32. | R. van Ee (2002). Voluntarily controlled percepts, will-power, and conscious vision. Journal of Vision, 2(10), 73. |
31. | A. Koene, K. Hol & R. van Ee (2002). Modeling curvature polarity in multi-stable 3D structure from motion. Perception, 31, S151. |
30. | L. C. J. van Dam & R. van Ee (2002). Stereoscopic matching and the aperture problem. Perception, 31, S159. |
29. | C. J. Erkelens, E. Gheorghiu & R. van Ee (2002). Stereopsis: the binding of depth to visual directions rather than to patterns. Perception, 31, S55. |
28. | R. van Ee & L. C. J. van Dam (2002). The influence of cyclovergence on unconstrained stereoscopic matching. Perception, 31, S55. |
27. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (2002). Conscious selection of bi-stable 3D percepts described by neural population codes. Journal of Vision, 2(7), S459a. |
2001 | |
26. | C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2001). Evidence for two binocular colour mechanisms. Journal of Vision, 1(3), S171a. |
25. | R. van Ee, K. Hol & C. J. Erkelens (2001). Bistable stereoscopic percepts and depth cue combination. Perception, 30, S42. |
24. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (2001). Attention-driven bistable stereoscopic slant percepts: Feedback versus cue-weighting mechanism. Society for NeuroScience, 722.24. |
23. | E. Berends, R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (2001). Differing roles of vertical disparity in perceiving direction and perceiving depth. Perception, 30, S43. |
22. | C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2001). Colour illusions in half-occlusions. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 42, S50. |
2000 and before | |
21. | R. van Ee & Anderson, B. L. (2000). The role of motion direction and speed in stereoscopic matching. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 41, S948. |
20. | R. van Ee, W. Richards, A. J. van Doorn & J.J. Koenderink (2000). Stereoanomaly explains inter-subject differences in various stereoscopic depth experiments. Perception, 29, S103. |
19. | E. Berends, C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2000). Adaptation to oscillating disparity. Perception, 29, S27. |
18. | C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (2000). Binocular colour mixing in half-occlusions. Perception, 29, S100. |
17. | W. J. Adams, M. S. Banks & R. van Ee (1999) Disparity adaptation following prolonged use of aniseikonic lenses. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 40, S193. |
16. | R. van Ee, M.S. Banks & B.T. Backus (1998). Perceived visual direction near an occluder. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 39, S621. |
15. | J.M. Hillis, B.T. Backus, R. van Ee & M.S. Banks (1998). Stable stereopsis derived from retinal signals and noisy oculomotor signals. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 39, S623. |
14. | C. Hung, R. van Ee & M.S. Banks (1998). Binocular visual direction of horizontal stimuli near an occluder. American Academy of Optometry Annual meeting, 39, S623. |
13. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (1997). Sequential fixation in stereoscopic slant estimation. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 38, S955. |
12. | C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (1997). Are stereopsis and ocular vergence based on headcentric disparity? Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 38, S621. |
11. | C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (1997). Stable stereopsis derived from retinal signals and noisy oculomotor signals. ATR Workshop on Human Visual Perception, Kyoto, Japan. |
10. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (1996). Binocularly perceived direction and outline shape. Perception, 25, p.70. |
9. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (1996). Binocular slant perception from scale, shear, rotation and divergence. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 37, S289. |
8. | M. S. Banks, R. van Ee & B. T. Backus (1996). Visual direction behind and near an occluder. Perception, 25, p.41. |
7. | B. T. Backus, R. van Ee & M. S. Banks (1996). Horizontal disparity pooling. Perception, 25, p.67. |
6. | C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (1996). Capture of Binocular Visual Direction. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 37, S290. |
5. | R. van Ee, A. J. M. Muijs & C. J. Erkelens (1995). Binocular vision: A circular disk occludes an elliptical area. International Conference on Visual Coding, Toronto, Canada. |
4. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (1995). A new interpretation of Werner's (1937) depth contrast effect. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 36, S367. |
3. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (1994). Time-dependent perception of large-field slants: validation of a model for depth perception during dynamic viewing. Perception, 23, p.35. |
2. | C. J. Erkelens & R. van Ee (1993). Disparity processing for depth perception under dynamic viewing conditions. Int. Conf. and Nato Adv. Workshop on Binocular Stereopsis and Optic Flow, Toronto, Canada. |
1. | R. van Ee & C. J. Erkelens (1993). Perception of local slant in relation to the geometry of binocular vision. Int. Conf. and Nato Adv. Workshop on Binocular Stereopsis and Optic Flow, Toronto, Canada. |
Technical Reports
R. van Ee and C.J. Erkelens (1993). Consequences of stable depth perception under natural viewing conditions for the processing of disparity. UBI-TR-93.MF-05. |
R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1993). Perception of slant in relation to the geometry of binocular vision. UBI-TR-93.MF-07. |
R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1995). Fundamental properties of binocular disparity as revealed by Helmholtz's coordinate system. UU-PA-PMI-028. |
R. van Ee & C.J. Erkelens (1995). On the stability of stereopsis under ego-movements: Binocular disparity calculations in Fick coordinates. UU-PA-PMI-036. |
R. van Ee (1995). On a learning effect in binocular slant perception. UU-PA-PMI-038. |
R. van Ee (1995). A possible explanation for Werner's binocular depth-contrast effect. UU-PA-PMI-039. |
Excluded from this list are technical reports that either became a full paper or were written as a co-author |